'TRUTH' DRUGS IN INTERROGATION

Document Type: 
Collection: 
Document Number (FOIA) /ESDN (CREST): 
CIA-RDP78-04491A000200020001-0
Release Decision: 
RIPPUB
Original Classification: 
K
Document Page Count: 
20
Document Creation Date: 
November 16, 2016
Document Release Date: 
May 17, 2000
Sequence Number: 
1
Case Number: 
Publication Date: 
January 1, 1961
Content Type: 
REPORT
File: 
AttachmentSize
PDF icon CIA-RDP78-04491A000200020001-0.pdf1.06 MB
Body: 
/ ' / Approved For Release 2000/06/05 : CIA-RDP78-04491A000200@20001-0 Effects of narcosis and consider- ations relevant to its possible counterintelligence use. "TRUTH" DRUGS IN INTERROGATION The search for effective aids to interrogation is probably as old as man's need to obtain information from an unco- operative source and as persistent as his impatience to short- cut any tortuous path. In the annals of police investigation, physical coercion has at times been substituted for painstak- ing and time-consuming inquiry in the belief that direct meth- ods produce quick results. Sir James Stephens, writing in 1883, rationalizes a grisly example of "third degree" practices by the police of India: "It is far pleasanter to sit comfort- ably in the shade rubbing red pepper in a poor devil's eyes than to go about in the sun hunting up evidence." More recently, police officials in some countries have turned to drugs for assistance in extracting confessions from accused persons, drugs which are presumed to relax the individual's defenses to the point that he unknowingly reveals truths he has been trying to conceal. This investigative technique, however humanitarian as an alternative to physical torture, still raises serious questions of individual rights and liberties. In this country, where drugs have gained only marginal ac- ceptance in police work, their use has provoked cries of "psy- chological third degree" and has precipitated medico-legal con- troversies that after a quarter of a century still occasionally flare into the open. The use of so-called "truth" drugs in police work is simi- lar to the accepted psychiatric practice of narco-analysis; the difference in the two procedures lies in their different objec- tives. The police investigator is concerned with empirical truth that may be used against the suspect, and therefore almost solely with probative truth: the usefulness of the sus- pect's revelations depends ultimately on their acceptance in evidence by a court of law. The psychiatrist, on the other hand, using the same "truth" drugs in diagnosis and treat- ment of the mentally ill, is primarily concerned with psy- chological truth or psychological reality rather than empirical CCC R!V ?ATE/// Z_ b BY W l A ?WAYPa 2etea 6 g 5TY~ l~' - 4491A000200020001-0 W CLASS REV CLASS JUST _ _ NEXT REV AUTH: HR 10.2 Approved For Release 2000/06/05 : CIA-RDP78-04491A000200020001-0 "Truth" Drugs fact. A patient's aberrations are reality for him at the time they occur, and an accurate account of these fantasies and delusions, rather than reliable recollection of past events, can be the key to recovery. The notion of drugs capable of illuminating hidden re- cesses of the mind, helping to heal the mentally ill and pre- venting or reversing the miscarriage of justice, has provided an exceedingly durable theme for the press and popular lit- erature. While acknowledging that "truth serum" is a mis- nomer twice over-the drugs are not sera and they do not necessarily bring forth probative truth-journalistic accounts continue to exploit the appeal of the term. The formula is to play up a few spectacular "truth" drug successes and to imply that the drugs are more maligned than need be and more widely employed in criminal investigation than can of- ficially be admitted. Any technique that promises an increment of success in ex- tracting information from an uncompliant source is ipso facto of interest in intelligence operations. If the ethical considera- tions which in Western countries inhibit the use of narco- interrogation in police work are felt also in intelligence, the Western services must at least be prepared against its pos- sible employment by the adversary. An understanding of "truth" drugs, their characteristic actions, and their poten- tialities, positive and negative, for eliciting useful informa- tion is fundamental to an adequate defense against them. This discussion, meant to help toward such an understand- ing, draws primarily upon openly published materials. It has the limitations of projecting from criminal investigative prac- tices and from the permissive atmosphere of drug psycho- therapy. Scopolamine as "Truth Serum" Early in this century physicians began to employ scopola- mine, along with morphine and chloroform, to induce a state of "twilight sleep" during childbirth. A constituent of hen- bane, scopolamine was known to produce sedation and drowsi- ness, confusion and disorientation, incoordination, and am- nesia for events experienced during intoxication. Yet physi- cians noted that women in twilight sleep answered questions accurately and often volunteered exceedingly candid remarks. ApprovQ For Release 2000/06/05: CIA-RDP78-04491A000200020001-0 Approved For Release 2000/06/05 : CIA-RDP78-04491A000200020001-0 "Truth" Drugs In 1922 it occurred to Robert House, a Dallas, Texas, ob- stetrician, that a similar technique might be employed in the interrogation of suspected criminals, and he arranged to in- terview under scopolamine two prisoners in the Dallas county jail whose guilt seemed clearly confirmed. Under the drug, both men denied the charges on which they were held; and both, upon trial, were found not guilty. Enthusiastic at this success, House concluded that a patient under the influence of scopolamine "cannot create a lie . . . and there is no power to think or reason." 14 His experiment and this conclusion attracted wide attention, and the idea of a "truth" drug was thus launched upon the public consciousness. The phrase "truth serum" is believed to have appeared first in a news report of House's experiment in the Los Angeles Record, sometime in 1922. House resisted the term for a while but eventually came to employ it regularly himself. He pub- lished some eleven articles on scopolamine in the years 1921- 1929, with a noticeable increase in polemical zeal as time went on. What had begun as something of a scientific statement turned finally into a dedicated crusade by the "father of truth serum" on behalf of his offspring, wherein he was "grossly indulgent of its wayward behavior and stubbornly proud of its minor achievements." 11 Only a handful of cases in which scopolamine was used for police interrogation came to public notice, though there is evidence suggesting that some police forces may have used it extensively.2. 16 One police writer claims that the threat of scopolamine interrogation has been effective in extracting confessions from criminal suspects, who are told they will first be rendered unconscious by chloral hydrate placed co- vertly in their coffee or drinking water.'? Because of a number of undesirable side effects, scopolamine was shortly disqualified as a "truth" drug. Among the most disabling of the side effects are hallucinations, disturbed per- ception, somnolence, and physiological phenomena such, as headache, rapid heart, and blurred vision, which distract: the subject from the central purpose of the interview. Further- more, the physical action is long, far outlasting the psycho- logical effects. Scopolomine continues, in some cases, to make anesthesia and surgery safer by drying the mouth and throat Approved For Release 2000/06/05 : CIA-RDP78-04491A000200020001-0 A3 Approved For Release 2000/06/05 : CIA-RDP78-04491A000200020001-0 "Truth" Drugs and reducing secretions that might obstruct the air passages. But the fantastically, almost painfully, dry "desert" mouth brought on by the drug is hardly conducive to free talking, even in a tractable subject. The Barbiturates The first suggestion that drugs might facilitate communi- cation with emotionally disturbed patients came quite! by ac- cident in 1916. Arthur S. Lovenhart and his associates at the University of Wisconsin, experimenting with respiratory stimulants, were surprised when, after an injection of sodium cyanide, a catatonic patient who had long been mute and rigid suddenly relaxed, opened his eyes, and even answered a few questions. By the early 1930's a number of psychiatrists were experimenting with drugs as an adjunct to established methods of therapy. At about this time police officials, still attracted by the pos- sibility that drugs might help in the interrogation of suspects and witnesses, turned to a class of depressant drugs known as the barbiturates. By 1935 Clarence W. Muehlberge:r, head of the Michigan Crime Detection Laboratory at East Lansing, was using barbiturates on reluctant suspects, though police work continued to be hampered by the courts' rejection of drug-induced confessions except in a few carefully circum- scribed instances. The, barbiturates, first synthesized in 1903, are among the oldest of modern drugs and the most versatile of all depres- sants. In this half-century some 2,500 have been prepared, and about two dozen of these have won an important place in medicine. An estimated three to four billion doses of bar- biturates are prescribed by physicians in the United States each year, and they have come to be known by a variety of commercial names and colorful slang expressions: "goofballs," Luminal, Nembutal, "red devils," "yellow jackets," "pink la- dies," etc. Three of them which are used in narcoanalysis and have seen service as "truth" drugs are sodium amytal (amobarbital), pentothal sodium (thiopental), and to a lesser extent seconal (secobarbital). As with most drugs, little is known about the way bar- biturates work or exactly how their action is related to their chemistry. But a great deal is known about the action it- Approvaec#For Release 2000/06/05 : CIA-RDP78-04491A000200020001-0 Approved For Release 2000/06/05 : CIA-RDP78-04491A00020002QfOu-O,, Drugs self. They can produce the entire range of depressant effects from mild sedation to deep anesthesia-and death. In small doses they are sedatives acting to reduce anxiety and respon- siveness to stressful situations; in these low doses, the drugs have been used in the treatment of many diseases, including peptic ulcer, high blood pressure, and various psychogenic dis- orders. At three to five times the sedative dose the same barbiturates are hypnotics and induce sleep or unconscious- ness from which the subject can be aroused. In larger doses a barbiturate acts as an anesthetic, depressing the central nervous system as completely as a gaseous anesthetic does. In even larger doses barbiturates cause death by stopping: respiration. The barbiturates affect higher brain centers generally. The cerebral cortex-that region of the cerebrum commonly thought to be of the most recent evolutionary development and the center of the most complex mental activities-seems to yield first to the disturbance of nerve-tissue function brought about by the drugs. Actually, there is reason to be- lieve that the drugs depress cell function without discrimina- tion and that their selective action on the higher brain'-cen- ters is due to the intricate functional relationship of cells in the central nervous system. Where there are chains of inter- dependent cells, the drugs appear to have their most pro- nounced effects on the most complex chains, those, control- ling the most "human" functions. The lowest doses of barbiturates impair the functioning of the cerebral cortex by disabling the ascending (sensory) cir- cuits of the nervous system. This occurs early in the seda- tion stage and has a calming effect not. unlike a drink or two after dinner. The subject is less responsive to stimuli. At higher dosages, the cortex no longer actively integrates in- formation, and the cerebellum, the "lesser brain" sometimes called the great modulator of nervous function, ceases to per- form as a control box. It no longer compares cerebral out- put with input, no longer informs the cerebrum command centers of necessary corrections, and fails to generate correct- ing command signals itself. The subject may become hyper- active, may thrash about. At this stage consciousness is lost and coma follows. The subject no longer responds even to A5 Approved For Release 2000/06/05 : CIA-RDP78-04491A000200020001-0 Approved For Release 2000/06/05 : CIA-RDP78-04491A000200020001-0 "Truth" Drugs noxious stimuli, and cannot be roused. Finally, in the last stage, respiration ceases.' 0. 28 As one pharmacologist explains it, a subject coming under the influence of a barbiturate injected intravenously goes through all the stages of progressive drunkenness, but the time scale is on the order of minutes instead of hours. Out- wardly the sedation effect is dramatic, especially if the sub- ject is a psychiatric patient in tension. His features slacken, his body relaxes. Some people are momentarily excited; a few become silly and giggly. This usually passes, and most subjects fall asleep, emerging later in disoriented sem'. wake- fulness. The descent into narcosis and beyond with progressively larger doses can be divided as follows : I. Sedative stage. II. Unconsciousness, with exaggerated reflexes (hyper- active stage). III. Unconsciousness, without reflex even to painful stimuli. IV. Death. Whether all these stages can be distinguished in any given subject depends largely on the dose and the rapidity with which the drug is induced. In anesthesia, stages I and II may last only two or three seconds. The first or sedative stage can be further divided: Plane 1. No evident effect, or slight sedative effect. Plane 2. Cloudiness, calmness, amnesia. (Upon recovery, the subject will not remember what happened at this or "lower" planes or stages.) Plane 3. Slurred speech, old thought patterns disrupted, inability to integrate or learn new patterns. Poor coordination. Subject becomes unaware of painful stimuli. Plane 3 is the psychiatric "work" stage. It may last only a few minutes, but it can be extended by further slow in- jection of the drug. The usual practice is to bring the sub- ject quickly to Stage II and to conduct the interview as he passes back into the sedative stage on the way to full con- sciousness. Appr~ l For Release 2000/06/05 : CIA-RDP78-04491A000200020001-~ Approved For Release 2000/06/05 : CIA-RDP78-04491A000200020001-0 "Truth" Drugs Clinical and Experimental Studies The general abhorrence in Western countries for the use of chemical agents "to make people do things against their will" has precluded serious systematic study (at least as pub- lished openly) of the potentialities of drugs for interrogation. Louis A. Gottschalk, surveying their use in information-seek- ing interviews,13 cites 136 references; but only two touch upon the extraction of intelligence information, and one of these concludes merely that Russian techniques in interrogation and indoctrination are derived from age-old police methods and do not depend on the use of drugs. On the validity of con- fessions obtained with drugs, Gottschalk found only three pub- lished experimental studies that he deemed worth reporting. One of these reported experiments by D. P. Morris in which intravenous sodium amytal was helpful in detecting malin- gerers.22 The subjects, soldiers, were at first sullen, nega- tivistic, and non-productive under amytal, but as the inter- view proceeded they revealed the fact of and causes for their malingering. Usually the interviews turned up a neurotic or psychotic basis for the deception. The other two confession studies, being more relevant to the highly specialized, untouched area of drugs in intelligence interrogation, deserve more detailed review. Gerson and Victoroff 12 conducted amytal interviews with 17 neuropsychiatric patients, soldiers who had charges against them, at Tilton General Hospital, Fort Dix. First they were interviewed without amytal by a psychiatrist, who, neither ignoring nor stressing their situation as prisoners or suspects under scrutiny, urged each of them to discuss his social and family background, his army career, and his version of the charges pending against him. The patients were told only a few minutes in advance that narcoanalysis would be performed. The doctor was consid- erate, but positive and forthright. He indicated that they had no choice but to submit to the procedure. Their attitudes varied from unquestioning compliance to downright refusal. Each patient was brought to complete narcosis and per- mitted to sleep. As he became semiconscious and could be stimulated to speak, he was held in this stage with additional amytal while the questioning proceeded. He was questioned Approved For I elease 2000/06/05 : CIA-RDP78-04491A000200020001-0 A7 Approv or a ease - - - 'Truth" Drugs rst about innocuous matters from his background that he had discussed before receiving the drug. Whenever possible, he was manipulated into bringing up himself the charges pend- ing against him before being questioned about them. If he did this in a too fully conscious state, it proved more ef- ective to ask him to "talk about that later" and to inter- ose a topic that would diminish suspicion, delaying the in- terrogation on his criminal activity until he was back. in the roper stage of narcosis. The procedure differed from therapeutic narcoanalysis in everal ways: the setting, the type of patients, and the kind f "truth" sought. Also, the subjects were kept in Lwilight onsciousness longer than usual. This state proved richest In yield of admissions prejudicial to the subject. In it his peech was thick, mumbling, and disconnected, but his dis- retion was markedly reduced. This valuable interrogation eriod, lasting only five to ten minutes at a time, could be einduced by injecting more amytal and putting the patient ack to sleep. The interrogation technique varied from case to case ac- ording to background information about the patient, the eriousness of the charges, the patient's attitude under nar- osis, and his rapport with the doctor. Sometimes it was use- ul to pretend, as the patient grew more fully conscious, that e had already confessed during the amnestic period of the nterrogation, and to urge him, while his memory and sense f self-protection were still limited, to continue to elaborate he details of what he had "already described." When it was bvious that a subject was withholding the truth, his denials ere quickly passed over and ignored, and the key questions ould be reworded in a new approach. Several patients revealed fantasies, fears, and delusions ap- roaching delirium, much of which could readily be distin- uished from reality. But sometimes there was no way for he examiner to distinguish truth from fantasy except by eference to other sources. One subject claimed to have a hild that did not exist, another threatened to kill on sight stepfather who had been dead a year, and yet another con- essed to participating in a robbery when in fact he had only Purchased goods from the participants. Testimony concern- Approv Approved For Release 2000/06/05 : CIA-RDP78-04491A000200029i0uth ` Drugs ing dates and specific places was untrustworthy and often contradictory because of the patient's loss of time-sense. His veracity in citing names and events proved questionable. Be- cause of his confusion about actual events and what he otbr feared had happened, the patient at times man- aged to conceal the truth unintentionally. As the subject revived, he would become aware that he was being questioned about his secrets and, depending upon his personality, his fear of discovery, or the degree of his disil- lusionment with the doctor, grow negativistic, hostile, or physi- cally aggressive. Occasionally patients had to be forcibly re- strained during this period to prevent injury to themselves or others as the doctor continued to interrogate. Some pa- tients, moved by fierce and diffuse anger, the assumption that they had already been tricked into confessing, and a still limited sense of discretion, defiantly acknowledged their guilt and challenged the observer to "do something about it." As the excitement passed, some fell back on their original stories and others verified the confessed material. During the follow-up interview nine of the 17 admitted the validity of their confessions; eight repudiated their confessions and reaffirmed their earlier accounts. With respect to the reliability of the results of such in- terrogation, Gerson and Victoroff conclude that persistent, careful questioning can reduce ambiguities in drug interroga- tion, but cannot eliminate them altogether. At least one experiment has shown that subjects are capa- ble of maintaining a lie while under the influence of a bar- biturate. Redlich and his associates at Yale 25 administered sodium amytal to nine volunteers, students and professionals, who had previously, for purposes of the experiment, revealed shameful and guilt-producing episodes of their past and then invented false self-protective stories to cover them. In nearly every case the cover story retained some elements of the guilt inherent in the true story. Under the influence of the drug, the subjects were cross- examined on their cover stories by a second investigator. The results, though not definitive, showed that normal individuals who had good defenses and no overt pathological traits could stick to their invented stories and refuse confession. Neu- A9 Approved For 4lease 2000/06/05 : CIA-RDP78-04491A000200020001-0 Appro Appro ed For Release 2000/06/05 : CIA-RDP78-04491A000200020001-0 "Truth" Drugs gtIiC'I ti viduals with strong unconscious self-punitive tend- encies, on the other hand, both confessed more easily and were inclined to substitute fantasy for the truth, confessing to offenses never actually committed. In recent years drug therapy has made some use of stimu- lants, most notably amphetamine (Benzedrine) and its rela- tive methamphetamine (Methedrine). These drugs, used either alone or following intravenous barbiturates, produce an outpouring of ideas, emotions, and memories which has been of help in diagnosing mental disorders. The potential of stimulants in interrogation has received little attention, unless in unpublished work. In one study of their psychiatric use Brussel et al.7 maintain that methedrine gives the liar no time to think or to organize his deceptions. Once the drug takes hold, they say, an insurmountable urge to pour out speech traps the malingerer. Gottschalk, on the other hand, says that this claim is extravagant, asserting without elaboration that the study lacked proper controls.13 It is evi- dent that the combined use of barbiturates and stimulants, perhaps along with ataraxics (tranquillizers), should be fur- ther explored. Observations from Practice J. M. MacDonald, who as a psychiatrist for the District Courts of Denver has had extensive experience with narco- analysis, says that drug interrogation is of doubtful value in obtaining confessions to crimes. Criminal suspects under the influence of barbiturates may deliberately withhold in- formation, persist in giving untruthful answers, or falsely confess to crimes they did not commit. The psychopathic personality, in particular, appears to resist successfully the influence of drugs. MacDonald tells of a criminal psychopath who, having agreed to narco-interrogation, received 1.5 grams of sodium amytal over a period of five hours. This man feigned amnesia and gave a false account of a murder. "He displayed little or no remorse as he (falsely) described the crime, including burial f the body. Indeed he was very self-possessed and he ap- eared almost to enjoy the examination. From time to time 1e would request that more amytal be injected." 21 A J or Release 2000/06/05 : CIA-RDP78-04491A000200020001-0 Approved For Release 2000/06/05 : CIA-RDP78-04491A000200029.0fi-Q Drugs MacDonald concludes that a person who gives false infor- mation prior to receiving drugs is likely to give false informa- under narcosis, that the drugs are of little value rYiing g deceptions, and that they are more effective in eleasing unconsciously repressed material than in evoking onsciously suppressed information. Another psychiatrist known for his work with criminals, L. Z. Freedman, gave sodium amytal to men accused of vari- ous civil and military antisocial acts. The subjects were men- ally unstable, their conditions ranging from character dis- orders to neuroses and psychoses. The drug interviews proved psychiatrically beneficial to the patients, but Freedman found that his view of objective reality was seldom improved by heir revelations. He was unable to say on the basis of he narco-interrogation whether a given act had or had not occurred. Like MacDonald, he found that psychopathic in- dividuals can deny to the point of unconsciousness crimes that every objective sign indicates they have committed.10 F. G. Inbau, Professor of Law at Northwestern University, who has had considerable experience observing and participat- ing in "truth" drug tests, claims that they are occasionally effective on persons who would have disclosed the truth any- ay had they been properly interrogated, but that a person determined to lie will usually be able to continue the decep- tion under drugs. The two military psychiatrists who made the most exten- ive use of narcoanalysis during the war years, Roy R. rinker and John C. Spiegel, concluded that in almost all cases they could obtain from their patients essentially the same material and give them the same emotional release by therapy without the use of drugs, provided they had sufficient time. The essence of these comments from professionals of long experience is that drugs provide rapid access to information that is psychiatrically useful but of doubtful validity as em- pirical truth. The same psychological information and a less adulterated empirical truth can be obtained from fully con- scious subjects through non-drug psychotherapy and skillful police interrogation. All Approved For F~elease 2000/06/05 : CIA-RDP78-04491A000200020001-0 Approv Approv 'Truth" Drugs p ajgn to CI Interrogation The almost total absence of controlled experimental studies )f "truth" drugs and the spotty and anecdotal nature of sychiatric and police evidence require that extrapolations to intelligence operations be made with care. Still, enough s known about the drugs' action to suggest certain considera ions affecting the possibilities for their use in interrogations. It should be clear from the foregoing that at best a drug an only serve as an aid to an interrogator who has a sure nderstanding of the psychology and techniques of normal nterrogation. In some respects, indeed, the demands on his kill will be increased by the baffling mixture of truth and antasy in drug-induced output. And the tendency against which he must guard in the interrogatee to give the responses hat seem to be wanted without regard for facts will be eightened by drugs: the literature abounds with warnings hat a subject in narcosis is extremely suggestible. It seems possible that this suggestibility and the lowered ward of the narcotic state might be put to advantage in he case of a subject feigning ignorance of a language or some ther skill that had become automatic with him. Lipton 20 ound sodium amytal helpful in determining whether a for- ign subject was merely pretending not to understand Eng- ish. By extension, one can guess that a drugged interro- atee might have difficulty maintaining the pretense that he lid not comprehend the idiom of a profession he was trying o hide. There is the further problem of hostility in the interro- ator's relationship to a resistance source. The accumulated nowledge about "truth" drug reaction has come largely from atient-physician relationships of trust and confidence. The ubject in narcoanalysis is usually motivated a priori to co- perate with the psychiatrist, either to obtain relief from mental suffering or to contribute to a scientific study. Even n police work, where an atmosphere of anxiety and. threat nay be dominant, a relationship of trust frequently asserts tself: the drug is administered by a medical man bound by a trict code of ethics; the suspect agreeing to undergo narco- nalysis in a ,desperate bid for corroboration of his testimony rusts both drug and psychiatrist, however apprehensively; l1 'For Release 2000/06/05 : CIA-RDP78-04491A000200020001-0 Approved For Release 2000/06/05 : CIA-RDP78-04491A000200020001-0 "Truth" Drugs and finally, as Freedman and MacDonald have indicated, the police psychiatrist frequently deals with a "sick" criminal, and some order of patient-physician relationship necessarily evolves. F11RGIThas a drug interrogation involved "normal" in- dividuals in a hostile or genuinely threatening milieu. It was from a non-threatening experimental setting that Eric Linde- mann could say that his "normal" subjects "reported a gen- eral sense of euphoria, ease and confidence, and they exhibited a marked increase in talkativeness and communicability." is Gerson and Victoroff list poor doctor-patient rapport as one factor interfering with the completeness and authenticity of confessions by the Fort Dix soldiers, caught as they were in a command performance and told they had no choice but to submit to narco-interrogation. From all indications, subject-interrogator rapport is usually crucial to obtaining the psychological release which may lead to unguarded disclosures. Role-playing on the part of the interrogator might be a possible solution to the problem of establishing rapport with a drugged subject. In therapy, the British narcoanalyst William Sargant recommends that the therapist deliberately distort the facts of the patient's life-experience to achieve heightened emotional response and abreaction.27 In the drunken state of narcoanalysis patients are prone to accept the therapist's false constructions. There is reason to expect that a drugged subject would communi- cate freely with an interrogator playing the role of relative, colleague, physician, immediate superior, or any other person to whom his background indicated he would be responsive. Even when rapport is poor, however, there remains one facet of drug action eminently exploitable in interrogation- the fact that subjects emerge from narcosis feeling they have revealed a great deal, even when they have not. As Gerson and Victoroff demonstrated at Fort Dix, this psychological set provides a major opening for obtaining genuine confes- sions. Technical Considerations It would presumably be sometimes desirable that a resist- ant interrogates be given the drug without his knowledge. For narcoanalysis the only method of administration used is Approved For Release 2000/06/05 : CIA-RDP78-04491A000200020001-0 A13 Approve For Release 2000/06/05 : CIA-RDP78-04491A000200020001-0 Trufh" Drugs Pr' & injection. The possibilities for covert or "silent" dministration by this means would be severely limited ex- ept in a hospital setting, where any pretext for intravenous njection, from glucose feeding to anesthetic procedure, could e used to cover it. Sodium amytal can be given orally, and he taste can be hidden in chocolate syrup, for example, but here is no good information on what dosages can be mmasked. oreover, although the drug might be introduced thus with- ut detection, it would be difficult to achieve and maintain he proper dose using the oral route. Administering a sterile injection is a procedure shor':ly mas- ered, and in fact the technical skills of intravenous injec- ion are taught to nurses and hospital corpsmen as a mat- er of routine. But it should be apparent that there is more o narcotizing than the injection of the correct amount of odium amytal or pentothal sodium. Administering drugs nd knowing when a subject is "under" require clinical judg- ent. Knowing what to expect and how to react a 3propri- tely to the unexpected takes both technical and clinical kill. The process calls for qualified medical personnel, and ober reflection on the depths of barbituric anesthesia will onfirm that it would not be enough merely to have access o a local physician. I ossible Variations In studies by Beecher and his associates,3-6 one-third to one- alf the individuals tested proved to be placebo reactors, sub- cts who respond with symptomatic relief to the administra- ion of any syringe, pill, or capsule, regardless of what; it con- ains. Although no studies are known to have been made I f the placebo phenomenon as applied to narco-interrogation, seems reasonable that when a subject's sense of guilt inter- res with productive interrogation, a placebo for pseudo-nar- osis could have the effect of absolving him of the responsibility r his acts and thus clear the way for free communication. It is notable that placebos are most likely to be effe 3tive in tuations of stress. The individuals most likely to react to lacebos are the more anxious, more self-centered, more de- endent on outside stimulation, those who express their needs more freely socially, talkers who drain off anxiety by con- ersing with others. The non-reactors are those clinically Approve I1 or Release 2000/06/05 : CIA-RDP78-04491 A000200020001-0 Approved For Release 2000/06/05 : CIA-RDP78-04491A000200020001-0 "Truth" Drugs more rigid and with better than average emotional control. No sex or I.Q. differences between reactors and non-reactors have been found. o possibility might be the combined use of drugs it otic trance and post-hypnotic suggestion: hypnosis could presumably prevent any recollection of the drug experi- ence. Whether a subject can be brought to trance against his will or unaware, however, is a matter of some disagree- ment. Orne, in a survey of the potential uses of hypnosis in interrogation,23 asserts that it is doubtful, despite many apparent indications to the contrary, that trance can be in- duced in resistant subjects. It may be possible, he adds, to hypnotize a subject unaware, but this would require a posi- tive relationship with the hypnotist not likely to be found in the interrogation setting. In medical hypnosis, pentothal sodium is sometimes em- ployed when only light trance has been induced and deeper narcosis is desired. This procedure is a possibility for inter- rogation, but if a satisfactory level of narcosis could be achieved through hypnotic trance there would appear to be no need for drugs. Defensive, Measures There is. no known way of building tolerance for a "truth" drug without creating a disabling addiction, or of arresting the action of a barbiturate once induced. The only full safe- guard against narco-interrogation is to prevent the admin- istration of the drug. Short of this, the best defense is to make use of the same knowledge that suggests drugs for of- fensive operations: if a subject knows that on emerging from narcosis he will have an exaggerated notion of how much he has revealed he can better resolve to deny he has said any- thing. The disadvantages and shortcomings of drugs in offensive operations become positive features of the defense posture. A subject in narco-interrogation is intoxicated, wavering be- tween deep sleep and semi-wakefulness. His speech is garbled and irrational, the amount of output drastically diminished. Drugs disrupt established thought patterns, including the will to resist, but they do so indiscriminately and thus also in- terfere with the patterns of substantive information the in- Approved For R~lease 2000/06/05 : CIA-RDP78-04491A000200020001-0 A15 Approv Approv [d For Release 2000/06/05 : CIA-RDP78-04491A000200020001-0 "Truth" Drugs minor way as aids in psychotherapy. Since information obtained from a person in a psychotic rug state would be unrealistic, bizarre, and extremely diffl- een used to create experimental "psychotic states," and in rPrgox~ seeks. Even under the conditions most favorable or the interrogator, output will be contaminated by fantasy, listortion, and untruth. Possibly the most effective way to arm oneself against narco- nterrogation would be to undergo a "dry run." A trial drug nterrogation with output taped for playback would familiarize n individual with his own reactions to "truth" drugs, and his familiarity would help to reduce the effects of harass- nent by the interrogator before and after the drug has been dministered. From the viewpoint of the intelligence serv- ce, the trial exposure of a particular operative to drugs might rovide a rough benchmark for assessing the kind and amount if information he would divulge in narcosis. There may be concern over the possibility of drug addic- ion intentionally or accidentally induced by an adversary ervice. Most drugs will cause addiction with prolonged use, nd the barbiturates are no exception. In recent studies at he U.S. Public Health Service Hospital for addicts in Lexing- on, Ky., subjects received large doses of barbiturates over a eriod of months. Upon removal of the drug, they experi- nced acute withdrawal symptoms and behaved in every re- pect like chronic alcoholics. Because their action is extremely short, however, and be- muse there is little likelihood that they would be administered egularly over a prolonged period, barbiturate "truth" drugs resent slight risk of operational addiction. If the adversary ervice were intent on creating addiction in order to exploit withdrawal, it would have other, more rapid means of pro- ucing states as unpleasant as withdrawal symptoms. The hallucinatory and psychotomimetic drugs such as mescaline, marihuana, LSD-25, and microtine are sometimes .mistakenly associated with narcoanalytic interrogation. hese drugs distort the perception and interpretation. of the ensory input to the central nervous system and affect vision, udition, smell, the sensation of the size of body parts and heir position in space, etc. Mescaline and LSD-2 5 have (For Release 2000/06/05 : CIA-RDP78-04491A000200020001-0 Approved For Release 2000/06/05 : CIA-RDP78-04491A00020002QQ01th0, Drugs cult to assess, the self-administration of LSD-25, which is ef- fective in minute dosages, might in special circumstances of- fer an operative temporary protection against interrogation. Conceivably, on the other hand, an adversary service could use such drugs to. produce anxiety or terror in medically un- sophisticated subjects unable to distinguish drug-induced psychosis from actual insanity. An enlightened operative could not be thus frightened, however, knowing that the ef- fect of these hallucinogenic agents is transient in normal in- dividuals. Most broadly, there is evidence that drugs have least ef- fect on well-adjusted individuals with good defenses and good emotional control, and that anyone who can withstand the stress of competent interrogation in the waking state can do so in narcosis. The essential resources for resistance thus appear to lie within the individual. Conclusions The salient points that emerge from this discussion are the following. No such magic brew as the popular notion of truth serum exists. The barbiturates, by disrupting defensive patterns, may sometimes be helpful in interrogation, but even under the best conditions they will elicit an output contami- nated by deception, fantasy, garbled speech, etc. A major vulnerability they produce in the. subject is a tendency to be- lieve he has revealed more than he has. It is possible, how- ever, for both normal individuals and psychopaths to resist drug interrogation; it seems likely that any individual who can withstand ordinary intensive interrogation can hold out in narcosis. The best aid to a defense against narco-inter- rogation is foreknowledge of the process and its limitations. There is an acute need for controlled experimental studies of drug reaction, not only to depressants but also to stimulants and to combinations of depressants, stimulants, and ataraxics. REFERENCES Adams, E. Barbiturates. Sci. Am., Jan. 1958, 198 (1), 60-64. Barkham, J. Truth Drugs: The new crime solver. Coronet, Jan. 1951, 29, 72-76. 3. Beecher, H. K. Anesthesia. Sci. Am., Jan. 1957, 198, p. 70. 4. Beecher, H. K. Appraisal of drugs intended to alter subjective re- sponses, symptoms. J. Amer. Med. Assn., 1955,158,399-401. A17 Approved For Release 2000/06/05 : CIA-RDP78-04491A000200020001-0 Approved For Release 2000/06/05 : CIA-RDP78-04491A000200020001-0 "Truth" Drugs 5. Beecher, H. K. Evidence for increased effectiveness of placebos with increased stress. Amer. J. Physiol., 1956, 187, 163-169. 6. Beecher, H. K. Experimental pharmacology and measurement of the subjective response. Science, 1953, 116, 157-162. 7. Brussel, J. A., Wilson, D. C., Jr., & Shankel, L. W. The use of methedrine in psychiatric practice. Psychiat. Quart., 1954, 28, 381-394. 8. Delay, J. Pharmacologic explorations of the personality: narco- analysis and "methedrine" shock. Proc. Roy. Soc. Med., 1949, 42, 492-496. 9. deRopp, R. S. Drugs and the Mind. New York: Grove Press, Inc., 1960. 10. Freedman, L. Z. "Truth" drugs. Sci. Am., March 1960, 145-154. 11. Geis, G. In scopolamine veritas. The early history of drug-in- duced statements. J. of Crim. Law, Criminol. & Pol. Sri., Nov.- Dec. 1959, 50(4), 347-356. 12. Gerson, M. J., & Victoroff, V. Experimental investigation into the validity of confessions obtained under sodium amytal narcosis. J. Clin. and Exp. Psychopath., 1948, 9, 359-375. 13. Gottschalk, L. A. The use of drugs in information-seeking inter- views. Technical report #2, ARDC Study SR 177-D Contract AF 18(600) 1797. Dec. 1958. Bureau of Social Science Research, Inc. 14. House, R. E. The use of scopolamine in criminology. Texas St. J. of Med., 1922, 18, 259. 15. Houston, F. A preliminary investigation into abreaction comparing methedrine and sodium amytal with other methods. J. ment. Sci., 1952, 98, 707-710. 16. Inbau, F. G. Self-incrimination. Springfield: C. C. Thomas, 1950. 17. Kidd, W. R. Police interrogation. 1940. 18. Legal dose of truth. Newsweek, Feb. 23, 1959, 28. 19. Lindemann, E. Psychological changes in normal and abnormal in- dividuals under the influence of sodium amytal. Amer. J. Psychiat., 1932, 11, 1083-1091. 20. Lipton, E. L. The amytal interview. A review. Amer. Practit. Digest Treat., 1950, 1, 148-163. 21. MacDonald, J. M. Narcoanalysis and criminal law. Amer. J. Psychiat., 1954, 111, 283-288. 22. Morris, D. P. Intravenous barbiturates: an aid in the diagnosis and treatment of conversion hysteria and malingering. Mil. Surg., 1945, 96, 509-513. 23. Orne, M. T. The potential uses of hypnosis in interrogation. An evaluation. ARDC Study SR 177-D Contract AF 18(600) 1797, Dec. 1958. Bureau of Social Science Research, Inc. 24. Pelikan, E. W., & Kensler, C. J. Sedatives: Their pharmacology and uses. Reprint from The Medical Clinics of North America. W. B. Saunders Company, Sept. 1958. 25. Redlich, F. C., Ravitz, L. J., & Dession, G. H. Narcoanalysis and truth. Amer. J.Psychiat., 1951, 107, 586-593. Approved For Release 2000/06/05 : CIA-RDP78-04491A000200020001-0 Approved For Release 2000/06/05 : CIA-RDP78-04491A000200020001-0 "Truth" Drugs 26. Rolin, J. Police Drugs. Translated by L. J. Bendit. New York: Philosophical Library, 1956. 27. Sargant, W., & Slater, E. Physical methods of treatment in psy- chiatry. (3rd ed.) Baltimore: Williams and Wilkins, 1954. 28. Snider, R. S. Cerebellum. Sci. Am., Aug. 1958, 84. 29. Uhr, L., & Miller, L. G. (eds.). Drugs and Behavior. New York- London: John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 1960. Approved For Release 2000/06/05 : CIA-RDP78-04491A000200020001-0 A19 25X1C Approved For Release 2000/06/05 : CIA-RDP78-04491A000200020001-0 Approved For Release 2000/06/05 : CIA-RDP78-04491A000200020001-0