PROPOSAL FOR RESEARCH SRI NO. ISU 75-124 SENSING OF REMOTE EM SOURCES (PHYSIOLOGICAL CORRELATES) PART TWO--TECHNICAL PROPOSAL

Document Type: 
Collection: 
Document Number (FOIA) /ESDN (CREST): 
CIA-RDP79-00999A000200010035-7
Release Decision: 
RIPPUB
Original Classification: 
K
Document Page Count: 
16
Document Creation Date: 
November 4, 2016
Document Release Date: 
January 9, 2014
Sequence Number: 
35
Case Number: 
Publication Date: 
June 3, 1975
Content Type: 
REPORT
File: 
AttachmentSize
PDF icon CIA-RDP79-00999A000200010035-7.pdf673.5 KB
Body: 
Declassified in Part - Sanitized Copy Approved for Release 2014/01/09: CIA-RDP79-00999A000200010035-7 0 - 4 STANFORD Menlo Park, RESEARCH California OGOUINUM 94025 U.S.A. Proposal for Research SRI No. ISU 75-124 SENSING OF REMOTE EM SOURCES (PHYSIOLOGICAL CORRELATES) Part One--Technical Proposal .Prepared for: ELEX 03X Naval Electronics Systems Command Washington, D.C. 20360 Attn: Mr. Paul Freund Approved: Air I t Earle D. Jones, Di/rector"' Electronics and B oengineering Laboratory 3 June 1975 Prepared by: Harold Puthoff Russell Targ Electronics and Bioengineering Laboratory eye Bonnar Cox, Executive Director Information Science and Engineering Division Declassified in Part - Sanitized Copy Approved for Release 2014/01/09: CIA-RDP79-00999A000200010035-7 Declassified in Part- Sanitized Copy Approved forRelease2014/01/09 : CIA-RDP79-00999A000200010035-7 Proposal for Research SRI No. ISU 75-124 SENSING OF REMOTE EM SOURCES (PHYSIOLOGICAL CORRELATES) I INTRODUCTION For the past three years we have had a program in the Electronics and Bioengineering Laboratory of SRI to investigate those characteristics of human perception which appear to fall outside the range of well- understood perceptual/processing capabilities. The phenomena of interest pertain to the ability of cert4in individuals to detect remote electro- magnetic stimuli which appear to be well shielded against detection. Of particular interest is a certain class of apparent coupling be- tween remote electromagnetic stimuli and the human nervous system as detected by the measurement of physiological responses, when overt re- sponses (e.g., verbal reports) provide no evidence for such registration. SRI proposes to undertake a one-year research program to investigate the characteristics of, and if possible to determine the mechanism re- sponsible for such coupling. 1 Declassified in Part - Sanitized Copy Approved for Release 2014/01/09: CIA-RDP79-00999A000200010035-7 Declassified in Part - Sanitized Copy Approved for Release 2014/01/09: CIA-RDP79-00999A000200010035-7 IT BACKGROUND In a number of laboratories evidence has been obtained indicating the existence of an as-yet-unidentified channel wherein information is observed to couple from remote electromagnetic stimuli to the human nervous system as indicated by physiological response, even though -overt responses such as verbalizations or key presses provide no evidence, for such information transfer. Physiological measures have included plethysmographic response' and EEG activity.23 Kamiya, Lindsley, Pribram, Silverman, Walter, and others have suggested that a whole range of EEG responses such as evoked potentials (EPs), spontaneous EEG, and the contingent negative variation (CNV) might be sensitive indicators of the detection ofremote stimuli not mediated by usual sensory processes.4 A pilot study was therefore undertaken at SRI to determine whether EEG activity could be used as a reliable indicator of information trans- mission between an isolated subject and a remote stimulus. Following the earlier work by others, we assumed that perception could be indicated by such a measure even in the absence of verbal or other overt indicators. With regard to choice of stimulus; it should be noted that Silver- man and Buchsbaum attempted, without success, to detect EP changes in a subject in response to a single stroboscopic flash stimulus observed by, another subject.6 Kamiya suggested that because of the unknown tem- poral characteristics of the informatipn channel, it might be more appropriate to use repetitive bursts of light to increase the probability of detecting information transfer.e, Therefore, in our study we chose to use repetitive light bursts as stimuli. The results, described below, have been reported in the open literature under the title "Information Transfer Under Conditions of Sensory Shielding," by R. Targ and H. Puthoff, Nature 252, 18 October 1974, and reprinted in the IEEE Communica- tions 13, January, 1975. In the design of the study it was assumed that the application of remote stimuli would result in responses similar to those obtained under conditions of direct stimulation. For example, when normal subjects are stimulated with a flashing light, their EEG typically shows a decrease in the amplitude of the resting rhythm and a driving of the brain waves at the fre- quency of the flashes.7 We hypothesized that if we stimulated 2 Declassified in Part - Sanitized Copy Approved for Release 2014/01/09: CIA-RDP79-00999A000200010035-7 Declassified in Part - Sanitized Copy Approved for Release 2014/01/09: CIA-RDP79-00999A000200010035-7 one subject in this manner (a putative sender), the EEG of another subject in a remote room with no flash present (a receiver), might show changes in alpha (9-11 Hz) activity, and possibly EEG driving similar to that of the sender, either by means of coupling to the sender's EEG, or by coupling directly to the stimulus. We informed our subject that at certain times a light was to be flashed in a sender's eyes in a distant room, and if the subject perceived that event, consciously or uncon- sciously, it might be evident from changes in his EEG output. The receiver was seated in a visually opaque, acoustically and electrically shielded double-walled steel room shown in Figure 1. The sender was seated in a room about 7 m from the receiver. We initially worked with four female and two male volunteer subjects. These were designated "receivers." The senders were either other subjects or the experimenters. We decided beforehand to run one or two sessions of 36 trials each with each subject in this selection procedure, and to do a more extensive study with any subject whose results were positive. A Grass PS-2 photostimulator placed about 1 m in front of the sender was used to present flash trains of 10 s duration. The receiver's EEG activity from the occipital region (Oz), referenced to linked mastoids, was amplified with a Grass 5P-1 preamplifier and associated driver amplifier with a bandpass of 1-120 Hz. The EEG data were recorded on magnetic tape with an Ampex SP 300 recorder. On each trial, a tone burst of fixed frequency was presented to both sender and receiver and was followed in one second by either a 10 s train of flashes or a null flash interval presented to the sender. Thirty-six such trials were given in an experi- mental session, consisting of 12 null trials--no flashes following the tone--12 trials of flashes at 6 f.p.s. and 12 trials of flashes at 16 f.p.s., all randomly intermixed, determined by entries from a table of random numbers. Each of the trials generated an 11-s EEG epoch. The last 4 s of the epoch was selected for analysis to minimize the desynchronising action of the warning cue. This 4-s segment was subjected to Fourier analysis on a LINC 8 computer. 3 Declassified in Part - Sanitized Copy Approved for Release 2014/01/09: CIA-RDP79-00999A000200010035-7 Declassified in Part - Sanitized Copy Approved for Release 2014/01/09: CIA-RDP79-00999A000200010035-7 LI L) FIGURE 1 SHIELDED ROOM USED FOR EEG EXPERIMENTS SA-2613-14 ( Declassified in Part - Sanitized Copy Approved for Release 2014/01/09: CIA-RDP79-00999A000200010035-7 Declassified in Part- Sanitized Copy Approved forRelease2014/01/09 : CIA-RDP79-00999A000200010035-7 Spectrum analyses gave no evidence of EEG driving in any re- ceiver, although in control runs the receivers did exhibit driving when physically stimulated with the flashes. But of the six subjects studied initially, one subject (1-1.11.) showed a consistent alpha blocking effect. We therefore undertook further study with this subject. Data from seven sets of 36 trials each were collected from this subject on three separate days. This comprises all the data collected to date with this subject under the test conditions described above. The alpha band was identified from average spectra, then scores Of average power and peak power were oh- tained from individual trials and subjected to statistical analysis. Of our six subjects, H.H. had by far the most monochromatic EEG spectrum. Figure 2 shows an overlay of the three averaged spectra from one of this subject's 36-trial runs, displaying changes in her alpha activity for the three stimulus conditions. Mean values for the.average power and peak power for each of the seven experimental sets are given in Table 1. The power measures were less in the 16 f.p.s. case than in the 0 f:p.s. in all seven peak power measures and in six out of seven average power measures. Note also the reduced effect in the case in which the subject was informed that no sender was present (Run 3). It seems that overall alpha production was reduced for this run in conjunction with the subject's ex- pressed apprehension about conducting the experiment without a sender.. This is in contrast to the case (Run 7) in which the subject was not informed. Siegel's two-tailed t approximation to the nonparametric randomi- zation tests was applied to the data from all sets, which in- cluded two sessions.. in which the sender was removed. Average power on trials associated with the occurrence of 16 f.p.s. was significantly less than when there were no flashes (t = 2.09, d.f. = 118-, P < 0.04). The second measure, peak power, was also significantly less in the 16 f.p.s. conditions than in the null condition (t = 2.16, d.f. = 118, P < 0.03). The average response in the 6 f.p.s. condition was in the same direction as that associated with 16 f.p.s., but the effect was not statistically. significant. As part of the experimental protocol the subject was asked to indicate conscious assessment for each trial as to 5 Declassified in Part - Sanitized Copy Approved for Release 2014/01/09: CIA-RDP79-00999A000200010035-7 Declassified in Part - Sanitized Copy Approved for Release 2014/01/09: CIA-RDP79-00999A000200010035-7 POTENTIAL (arbitrary units) ? ? ? 5 Hz 10 Hz 15 Hz THREE CASES ? 0, 6 and 16 Hz flashes (12 trial averages) SA-2613-15 FIGURE 2 OCCIPITAL EEG FREQUENCY SPECTRA, 0 TO 20 Hz, OF ONE SUBJECT (H.H.) ACTING AS RECEIVER, SHOWING AMPLITUDE CHANGES IN THE 9-11 Hz BAND AS A FUNCTION OF STROBE FREQUENCY 6 Declassified in Part - Sanitized Copy Approved for Release 2014/01/09: CIA-RDP79-00999A000200010035-7 Declassified in Part - Sanitized Copy Approved for Release 2014/01/09: CIA-RDP79-00999A000200010035-7 Table 1 EEG DATA FOR H.H. SHOWING AVERAGE POWER AND PEAK POWER IN. THE 19 11 Hz BAND, AS A FUNCTION OF FLASH FREQUENCY AND SENDER. EACH TABLE ENTRY IS AN AVERAGE OVER 12 TRIALS. Flash , Frequency Sender Average Power 0 6 16 Peak Power 0 6 16 J.L. 94.8 84.1 76.8 357.7 329.2 289.6 , R.T. 41.3 45.5 37.0 160.7 161.0 125.0 No Sender 25.1 35.7 28.2 87.5 95.7 81.7 (Subject informed) J.L. 54.2 55.3 44.8 191.4 170.5 149.3 J L .. 56.8 50.9 32.8 240.6 178.0 104.6 R.T. 39.8 24.9 30.3 145.2 74.2 122.1 No Sender 86.0 53.0 52.1 318.1 180.6 202.3 (Subject not informed) Averages 56.8 49.9 43.1 214.5 169.8 153-5 -127. -247 (P?.04) -217 -287 (P