A REVIEW OF COLOR SCIENCE AND COLOR AERIAL RECONNAISSANCE

Document Type: 
Document Number (FOIA) /ESDN (CREST): 
CIA-RDP78B04560A007300010030-5
Release Decision: 
RIPPUB
Original Classification: 
C
Document Page Count: 
186
Document Creation Date: 
December 28, 2016
Document Release Date: 
March 25, 2003
Sequence Number: 
30
Case Number: 
Publication Date: 
January 1, 1972
Content Type: 
REPORT
File: 
AttachmentSize
PDF icon CIA-RDP78B04560A007300010030-5.pdf7.38 MB
Body: 
Approved For Release 2003/05/14: CIA-RDP78BO456OA00730001003egNFIDENTIAL TECHNICAL PUBLICATION NATIONAL PHOTOGRAPHIC INTERPRETATION CENTER A REVIEW OF COLOR SCIENCE AND COLOR AERIAL RECONNAISSANCE CONFIDENTIAL Declass Review by NIMA / DoD NPIC/R-03/72 JANUARY 1972 GROUP Ii EXCLUDED FROM AUTOMATIC DOWNGRADING AND DECLASSIFICATION Approved For Release 2003/05/14: CIA-RDP78BO456OA007300010030-5 Approved For Release 2003/05/14: CIA-RDP78BO456OA007300010030-5 This document contains information affecting the national defense of the United States, within the meaning of Title 18, sections 793 and 794, of the U.S. Code, as amended. Its transmission or revelation of its contents to or receipt by an unauthorized person is prohibited by law. Approved For Release 2003/05/14: CIA-RDP78BO456OA007300010030-5 Approved For Release 2001 1 MIR:M[L78BO456OA00730001003QZ'R-03/72 A REVIEW OF COLOR SCIENCE AND COLOR AERIAL RECONNAISSANCE January 1972 Approved For Release 2OO iF4 PE A D L78BO456OA007300010030-5 Approved For Release 2003(Yf(F fD E78BO456OA007300010M5R-03/72 1.0 INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 1.1 PURPOSE AND SCOPE . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 1.2 FORMAT . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 2.0 THE PHYSIOLOGICAL AND PSYCHOLOGICAL ASPECTS OF COLOR VISION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3 2.1 THE ANATOMY AND FUNCTION OF THE VISUAL SYSTEM AS RELATED TO COLOR . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3 2.2 THE COLOR SENSITIVITY OF THE VISUAL SYSTEM . . . . . . . . 9 2.2.1 Luminous Range . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 9 2.2.2 Spectral Range . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 11 2.2.3 Geometric Extent of the Color Zones . . . . . . . . 13 2.3.1 Area Effect . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 16 2.3.2 Simultaneous Contrast and Edge Effects . . . . . . . 17 2.3.3 Spreading Effects . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 19 2.3.4 After Images . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 19 2.3.5 Color Constancy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 20 2.3.6 Irradiation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 21 2.3.6.1 Explanation of Irradiation . . . . . . . . . 21 2.3.6.2 Additional Complications of Mach Bands for Photointerpretation . . . . . . . . . . . . . 23 2.3.7 Adaptation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 24 2.4 DEPTH PERCEPTION AND COLOR . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 25 2.4.1 Perceptual Cues of Depth . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 25 2.4.1.1 Monocular Cues . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 25 2.4.1.2 Binocular Cues . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 26 2.4.2 Effect of Color on Cues . . . . . . . . . . . . 28 2.4.3 Chromastereopsis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 28 2.4.4 The Pulfrich Phenomenon . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 29 2.5 COLOR CAPABILITIES AND SKILLS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 29 Approved For Release 200t3W!U&T14?b78BO456OA007300010030-5 Approved For Release 2001&4~I 2 78BO456OA00730001OQ-5/R-03/72 TABLE OF CONTENTS (Continued) Page 2.5.1 Color Discrimination . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 29 2.5.2 Color Memory . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 30 2.5.3 Color Naming . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 32 2.5.4 Color Matching (see also 3.4 COLORIMETRY) . . . . . . 32 2.6.1 Defective Color Vision . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 34 2.6.1.1 Types of Color Vision Defects . . . . . . . . . 35 2.6.1.2 Congenital versus Acquired Color-Vision Defects . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 36 2.6.1.3 The Effects of Age on Color Perception . . . . 36 2.6.2 Color-Vision Tests . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 37 2.6.2.1 Pseudoisochromatic Chart Tests (PIC) . . . . . 37 2.6.2.2 The Farnsworth-Munsell 100-Hue Test (FMT) . . . 38 2.6.2.3 The Inter-Society Color Council Color Aptitude Test (ISCC-CAT) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 38 2.6.2.4 The Burnham-Clark-Munsell Color-Memory Test (BCMS) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 39 2.6.2.5 Campimeter Test . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 40 2.6.2.6 Explanation of Test Battery Usage . . . . . . . 40 2.7 REFERENCES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 41 3.0 THE PHYSICS OF COLOR . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 46 3.1 THE PHYSICAL ASPECTS OF COLOR 3.2 THE ADDITIVE AND SUBTRACTIVE CONCEPT OF COLOR REPRODUCTION 53 3.2.1 The Additive Method of Color Reproduction . . . . . . 54 3.2.2 The Subtractive Method of Color Reproduction . . . . . 54 3.3 COLOR SYSTEMS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 60 3.3.1 Munsell System . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 60 3.3.2 CIE System . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 62 3.3.3 Lovibond System . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 65 3.3.4 Ostwald System . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 65 3.3.5 DIN System . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 67 3.3.6 Densitometric Munsell System . . . . . . . . . . . . . 69 3.3.7 ISCC-NBS System . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 69 3.3.8 NuHue, Plochere, Ridgway, Maerz and Paul, Villalobos, Textile Color Card Association, and Methuin . . . . . 70 Approved For Release 200~1o#RI * 78BO456OA007300010030-5 Approved For Release 200 MAIM DTA 78BO456OA007300010M5/R-03/72 TABLE OF CONTENTS (Continued) Page 3.4 COLORIMETRY--THE MEASUREMENT OF COLOR . . . . . . . . . . . . 70 3.4.1 Instrumental Colorimetry . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 73 3.4.1.1 Recording Spectrophotometer . . . . . . . . . . 73 3.4.1.2 Tristimulus Colorimeter . . . . . . . . . . 75 3.4.2 Visual Colorimetry . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 75 3.4.3 Treatment of Colorimetric Data and Error Analysis . . 77 3.4.4 Metamerism and Metameric Colors . . . . . . . . . . . 78 3.4.5 Color Differences and Tolerances . . . . . . . . . . . 81 3.4.6 Color Rendering and Color-Rendering Indices . . . . . 83 3.5 COLOR DENSITOMETRY . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 83 3.5.1 Specular and Diffuse Density . . . . . . . . . . . . . 85 3.5.2 Analytical and Integral Densitometry . . . . . . . . . 85 3.6 REFERENCES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 89 4.0 COLOR AERIAL PHOTOGRAPHY . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 94 4.1 COLOR-FILM THEORY . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 94 4.2 PHOTOGRAPHIC PROPERTIES OF SELECTED AERIAL FILMS . . . . . . 99 4.2.1 Color Films . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 99 4.2.2 False-Color Films . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 101 4.2.3 Spectrazonal or Multispectral Film and Techniques . . 102 4.2.4 Additive Color Separations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 103 4.3 EFFECTS OF TARGET AND ACQUISITION PARAMETERS ON COLOR AERIAL PHOTOGRAPHY AND COLOR PERCEPTION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 103 4.3.1 Colorimetric Properties of Selected Natural and Man- Made Targets . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 103 4.3.2 Effects of the Atmosphere and Sun Angle . . . . . . . 108 4.3.3 Effects of Lenses and Lens Aberrations . . . . . . . . 114 4.3.4 Effects of Platform and Camera Vibration . . . . . . . 114 4.4 EFFECTS OF FILM AND PROCESSING PARAMETERS ON COLOR AERIAL PHOTOGRAPHY AND COLOR PERCEPTION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 114 Approved For Release 200$ONFIOH,- 78BO456OA007300010030-5 Approved For Release 200 TOO I: fj9 8BO456OA007300010Q 6/R-03/72 Page 4.4.1 Effects of Granularity . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 114 4.4.2 Effects of Film Variance . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 115 4.4.3 Effects of Processing Variance . . . . . . . . . . . . 116 5.0 APPLIED ASPECTS OF COLOR AND COLOR PERCEPTION IN IMAGE INTERPRETATION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 121 5.1.1 Theoretical Advantages of Color Films . . . . . . . . 122 5.1.2 Experimental Comparisons of Color Films With Black and White Films . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 123 5.1.3 Operational Findings on Color Film and Color-Imaging Techniques . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 123 5.1.3.1 Natural Color Films (see 4.2.1 Color Films) . . 124 5.1.3.1.1 Advantages . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 124 5.1.3.1.2 Disadvantages . . . . . . . . . . . . . 125 5.1.3.2 Color Infrared Film (Ektachrome Infrared 8443, See 4.2.2 False-Color Films) . . . . . . . . . 126 5.1.3.2.1 Advantages . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 126 5.1.3.2.2 Disadvantages . . . . . . . . . . . . . 127 5.1.3.3 Additive Color Separation (See 4.4.2 Additive Color Separations) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 127 5.1.3.3.1 Advantages . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 127 5.1.3.3.2 Disadvantages . . . . . . . . . . . . . 128 5.1.3.4 Spectrazonal or Multispectral Techniques (See 4.2.3 Spectrazonal or Multispectral Film and Techniques) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 129 5.1.3.4.1 Advantages . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 129 5.1.3.4.2 Disadvantages . . . . . 129 5.1.3.5 Black and White Infrared (Kodak Infrared Aerographic Film, See 4.2.4 False-Color Films) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 130 5.1.3.5.1 Advantages . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 130 5.1.3.5.2 Disadvantages . . . . . . . . . . . . . 130 5.1.4 Operational Findings on the Interpretation of Targets and Backgrounds on Color Films . . . . . . . . . . . . 130 5.1.4.1 Tactical Targets . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 131 5.1.4.2 Strategic Targets . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 132 5.1.4.3 Cultural Objects . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 133 5.1.4.4 Vegetation Types . . . . . . ... . . . . . . . 133 Approved For Release 200 @IRIDN-2 8BO456OA007300010030-5 Approved For Release 2001 DJIAl[378BO456OA007300010O8Q6'R-03/72 TABLE OF CONTENTS (Continued) Page 5.1.4.5 Soils . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 134 5.1.4.6 Water . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 134 5.1.4.7 Geologic Features . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 138 5.2 THE MENSURATION OF COLOR FILMS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 138 5.3 INTERPRETATION TECHNIQUES AND COLOR FILMS . . . . . . . . . . 139 5.3.1 Stereoscopic Viewing . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 139 5.3.2 Magnification . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 139 5.3.3 Scanning Strategies . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 140 5.3.4 Multisensor Viewing . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 140 5.3.5 Change Detection . . . . . . . . ... . . . . . . . . . 140 5.3.6 Reporting Strategies . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 141 5.4 ENHANCING THE INTERPRETATION OF COLOR FILMS . . . . . . . . . 143 5.4.1 Enhancement During Acquisition . . . . . . . . . . . . 143 5.4.1.1 Haze Filters . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 143 5.4.1.2 Antivignetting Filters . . . . . . . . . . . . 143 5.4.1.3 Narrow-Band Filters . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 144 5.4.2 Enhancement During Processing . . . . . . . . . . . . 144 5.4.2.1 Color Separation Negatives . . . . . . . . . . 144 5.4.2.2 Color Balance . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 144 5.4.3 Enhancement During Interpretation . . . . . . . . . . 144 5.4.3.1 Color Filters . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 145 5.4.3.2 Colored Lights . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 145 5.5 ILLUMINATION CONDITIONS AFFECTING THE PERCEPTION OF COLOR IMAGERY . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 145 5.5.1 Illuminant Specifications for Light Tables . . . . . . 145 5.5.1.1 Intensity . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 145 5.5.1.2 Spectral Distribution . . . . . . . . . . . . . 146 5.5.2 Illuminant Specifications for Ambient Lighting . . . . 146 5.6 REFERENCES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 147 6.0 GLOSSARY . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 156 7.0 SUGGESTED READINGS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 162 7.1 GENERAL REFERENCES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 162 Approved For Release 200 1[ N-T ]8B04560A007300010030-5 Approved For Release 2003 IU AlL8B0456OA0073000100$O /R-03/72 TABLE OF CONTENTS (Continued) Page 7.2 ANATOMY AND PHYSIOLOGY OF COLOR VISION . . . . . . . . . . . 163 7.3 PSYCHOLOGICAL AND PSYCHOPHYSICAL ASPECTS OF COLOR VISION 164 7.4 COLOR VISION TESTS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 165 7.5 PHYSICS OF COLOR . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 166 7.6 COLOR SYSTEMS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 167 7.7 COLORIMETRY AND COLOR DENSITOMETRY . . . . . . . . . . . . . 168 7.8 COLOR AERIAL RECONNAISSANCE . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 169 8.0 INDEX . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 170 FIGURE 2.1 HORIZONTAL SECTION OF THE EYE . . . . . . . . . . . . 5 FIGURE 2.2 THE STRUCTURE OF THE HUMAN RETINA . . . . . . . . . . 7 FIGURE 2.3 THE COURSE OF VISUAL STIMULI AND THE CORRESPONDING FIELDS OF VISION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 8 FIGURE 2.4 DISTRIBUTION OF THE FOVEAL PHOTOCHROMATIC INTERVAL . . 10 FIGURE 2.5 BEZOLD-BRUCKE PHENOMENON . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 11 FIGURE 2.6 PHOTOPIC LUMINOSITY FUNCTION (solid line) . . . . . . 14 FIGURE 2.7 THE GEOMETRIC COLOR ZONES OF THE EYE . . . . . . . . . 15 FIGURE 2.8 SIMULTANEOUS COLOR CONTRAST . . . . . . . . . . . . . 18 FIGURE 2.9 BRIGHTNESS CONTRAST . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 18 Approved For Release 200 3 1NfI ~b k8B04560A007300010030-5 Approved For Release 20tH/1 78BO456OA00730001dnO 6 -03/72 LIST OF FIGURES (Continued) Page FIGURE 2.10 ILLUSTRATION OF THE PHENOMENON OF MACH BANDS . . . . 22 FIGURE 2.11 ILLUSTRATION OF DEPTH PERCEPTION AND RETINAL DISPARITY . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 27 FIGURE 2.12 NUMBER OF JUST-PERCEPTIBLE STEPS BETWEEN NEUTRAL AND THE SPECTRUM COLORS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 31 FIGURE 2.13 DIFFERENTIAL COLOR SENSITIVITY . . . . . . . . . . . 31 FIGURE 3.1 THE ESSENTIAL ELEMENTS OF COLOR . . . . . . . . . . . 47 FIGURE 3.2 THE ELECTROMAGNETIC SPECTRUM . . . . . . . . . . . . 48 FIGURE 3.3 THE SPECTRAL ENERGY DISTRIBUTION FOR A 40-WATT INCANDESCENT LAMP . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 49 FIGURE 3.4 THE SPECTRAL ENERGY DISTRIBUTION FOR A 40-WATT DAYLIGHT FLUORESCENT LAMP . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 49 FIGURE 3.5 THE SPECTRAL ENERGY DISTRIBUTION OF THE LIGHT REACHING THE OBSERVER'S EYE . . . . . . . . . . . . . 51 FIGURE 3.6 THE COLOR STIMULUS EXPERIENCED BY THE OBSERVER . . . 52 FIGURE 3.7 ADDITIVE COLOR PHOTOGRAPHY - TAKING THE PICTURE . . . 55 FIGURE 3.8 ADDITIVE COLOR PHOTOGRAPHY - RECONSTRUCTING THE PICTURE . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 56 FIGURE 3.9 SPECTRAL TRANSMITTANCE CURVES FOR IDEALIZED CYAN, MAGENTA, AND YELLOW DYES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 57 FIGURE 3.10 A COLOR REVERSAL PHOTOGRAPHIC PROCESS . . . . . . . . 59 FIGURE 3.11 THE CONCEPT OF THE MUNSELL COLOR SYSTEM . . . . . . . 61 FIGURE 3.12 THE CIE COLOR SYSTEM . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 63 FIGURE 3.13 THE OSTWALD COLOR SYSTEM . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 66 FIGURE 3.14 THE LINES OF CONSTANT DIN-FARBTON AND DIN-SXTTIGUNG PLOTTED ON A 1931 CIE DIAGRAM . . . . . . . . . . . 68 Approved For Release 2006?NF4D 1TF t78BO456OA007300010030-5 Approved For Release 200tYM IA-M[L78BO456OA007300010R~QtS/R-03/72 LIST OF FIGURES (Continued) Page FIGURE 3.15 THE ISCC-NBS HUE NAMES AND ABBREVIATIONS FOR A CONSTANT MUNSELL VALUE OF SIX . . . . . . . . . . . . 71 FIGURE 3.16 THE ISCC-NBS MODIFIERS FOR A PURPLE,HUE . . . . . . . 72 FIGURE 3.17 A SCHEMATIC DIAGRAM OF A SPECTROPHOTOMETER BEING USED TO MEASURE THE SPECTRAL TRANSMITTANCE OF A SAMPLE . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 74 FIGURE 3.18 A SCHEMATIC DIAGRAM OF A TRISTIMULUS COLORIMETER . . 76 FIGURE 3.19 THE SPECTRAL REFLECTANCE CURVES FOR A METAMERIC PAIR OF COLORS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 79 FIGURE 3.20 THE SPECTRAL ENERGY DISTRIBUTIONS FOR THE CIE STANDARD SOURCES A AND C . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 79 FIGURE 3.21 THE SHIFT IN THE CIE CHROMATICITY COORDINATES OF A PAIR OF METAMERIC COLORS PRODUCED BY CHANGING FROM CIE STANDARD SOURCE C TO CIE STANDARD SOURCE A . . . 80 FIGURE 3.22 THE STANDARD DEVIATIONS OF COLOR MATCHES BY OBSERVER PGN, ENLARGED TEN TIMES ON THE 1931 CIE x, y CHROMATICITY DIAGRAM . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 82 FIGURE 3.23 A SCHEMATIC DIAGRAM OF A TYPICAL COLOR DENSITOMETER . 84 FIGURE 4.1 THE CONFIGURATION OF A TYPICAL COLOR FILM . . . . . . 95 FIGURE 4.2 THE FORMATION AND DEVELOPMENT OF A COLORED IMAGE IN A COLOR-REVERSAL PHOTOGRAPHIC PROCESS . . . . . . . . 97 FIGURE 4.3 CIE CHROMATICITY COORDINATES FOR SELECTED NATURAL AND MAN-MADE TARGETS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 104 FIGURE 4.4 IMAGE DISTORTION CAUSED BY A TURBULENT ATMOSPHERE . . 109 FIGURE 4.5 LIGHT LOSSES CAUSED BY BOTH ABSORPTION AND SCATTERING IN THE ATMOSPHERE . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 111 FIGURE 4.6 DIAGRAM OF THE SUN ANGLE OR SOLAR ALTITUDE AND THE RELATIVE POSITIONS OF THE EARTH, CAMERA PLATFORM, AND THE SUN . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 112 Approved For Release 20& hE! JP78BO456OA007300010030-5 Approved For Release 20Q3,fQr ofbapf 78B0456OA00730001@ $R-03/72 LIST OF FIGURES (Continued) Page FIGURE 4.7 VARIATION OF TARGET ILLUMINANCE AS A FUNCTION OF SUN ANGLE . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 113 TABLE 2.1 WAVELENGTH REGIONS AND HUE NAMES . . . . . . . . . . . 12 TABLE 4.1 THE PHOTOGRAPHIC CHARACTERISTICS OF SELECTED AERIAL COLOR FILMS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 100 TABLE 4.2 THE CIE CHROMATICITY COORDINATES, LIGHTNESSES, DOMINANT WAVELENGTHS AND EXCITATION PURITIES FOR THE COLORS OF SELECTED NATURAL AND CULTURAL TARGETS . . . . 105 TABLE 4.3 THE MUNSELL AND ISCC-NBC DESIGNATIONS FOR THE COLORS OF SELECTED NATURAL AND CULTURAL TARGETS . . . . . . . 107 TABLE 5.1 ACCURACY OF CROP IDENTIFICATION ON TRUE COLOR FILM . . 135 TABLE 5.2 SOILS OF THE LAC BEVIN BASIN AND THEIR MUNSELL VALUES . 142 Approved For Release 200 ' DIKT478BO456OA007300010030-5 Approved For Release 200:IONFIDNit78B04560A0073000100/R-03/72 This review attempts to explain in simple, understandable lan- guage the current state of the art of color science and color photography as it relates to high-altitude aerial reconnaissance, and the Center's activities. The review provides the Center with a reference (a) for use by the Center personnel in learning and understanding basic color concepts and technologies (it is not intended to be a textbook or a handbook), (b) to show how color will affect the Center, and (c) to show how color will be used by the Center. For maximum usefulness to the Center, several criteria were used to guide and limit the compilation and writing of the review: (1) The material included in the review should be relevant and applicable to the Center's activities and needs. The Center's activity is broad and many technical disciplines are represented. Thus the review is broad in scope, covering the psychology, physiology, and physics of color; color photography; and applied as ects of color imagery and interpretation . Materials within these broad categories, that were unrelated to the Center's activ- ities are not included. Thus, the reader should not expect to find a complete and comprehensive review. (2) The language (terms and concepts) of the review should be nontheoretical and easy to understand so that a wide range of Center personnel can read and understand the material included. For those who wish or need to know technical detail and theory, a number of excellent references in all technical areas are suggested in the report. (3) The material should be presented. in a straight- forward, concise manner so that extraneous words and concepts would not have to be read. (4) The material presented should be as up-to-date as possible and based on sound empirical or operationally proven evidence. * The classified information in this topic area is included in the report entitled "A Review of Color Science and Color Aerial Reconnaissance: An Addendum". Approved For Release 2001 ~IMN-T2 78BO456OA007300010030-5 Approved For Release 20tty+7HyP78BO456OA00730001U83Q1-03/72 (5) The material should be easily accessed. (To satisfy this criteria, numbered headings are used extensively and an index is included.) It should be noted that, as a result, the review is not an integrated whole, rather many sections stand alone. Cross-referencing between sections is used to help the reader gain a fuller under- standing of the material, in the review, pertaining to a particular topic area. To help satisfy these criteria the writers first performed a comprehensive literature review. Then, individuals working or researching in certain color-related fields were contacted to gather state of the art information. Every attempt has been made to avoid the use of unnecessary technical jargon, lengthy explanations, theoretical considerations, and yet, to give the reader an understanding of the basic terminologies and concepts used in color science and aerial photography. This review begins with the physiology and psychology of color vision, which is followed by the physics of color. Color aerial photography is discussed, and the body of the review ends with the applied aspects of color imagery and its interpretation. At the end of each major section is a partial list of references that are considered the most relevant and useful for the review. Within each section, the format of the sections varies to suit the material presented. In general, the format is continuous and explanatory, but in the last section dealing with applied aspects much of the information was disjointed and, thus, is presented in a more discrete fashion. To help the reader, a glossary and an index are included, and, within the body of the review, the glossary-defined words are capitalized and underlined. Important words, defined in the text are also capitalized. In addition, a suggested reading list has been included for the reader to find more detailed and theoretical discussions. This reading list is categorized by topic and level of technical difficulty. Approved For Release 20e"fil0 78BO456OA007300010030-5 Approved For Release 2003 ffIJ; Rt8B04560A0073000100805/R-03/72 2.0 THE PHYSIOLOGICAL AND PSYCHOLOGICAL ASPECTS OF COLOR VISION The influx of color imagery into the Center's operation will have at least one fundamental effect; eventually everyone will look at it (as transparencies or prints). The interpreters, printers, photogrammetrists, and audiences of briefings will be seeing color imagery. However, these people may not perceive color in the same way. Where some people see red, others may see orange. An interpreter viewing color imagery through a stereoscope will see colors differently than when he views without the scope. These are but a few examples of many problems which can be anticipated. The importance of these problems to the Center's activities and tasks is difficult to anticipate. 2.1 THE ANATOMY AND FUNCTION OF THE VISUAL SYSTEM AS RELATED TO COLOR A complete explanation of the physiological basis of color perception cannot be given by the present state of the art. Certain facts are known as to the necessary constituent elements, but how they interact to achieve color vision can be explained only by theories that are partially contradicatory. The purpose of the eye is to focus light rays onto the retina (the light sensitive layer of the eye) and to convert these rays from light energy into electrical-like impulses for transmission by the nervous system. The characteristics of the scene can then be passed up to the interpretative centers of the brain. Further, these characteristics must be coded in some way as to their spatial relationships, color, shades, contrast, etc. To do this, the eyes must be able to move, to focus, and to adapt to fluctuating illumination levels, as a coordinated pair. The ability of the eyes to move and point specifically at an object is a part of the basis for the relatively high resolution ability of the visual system, because good acuity as well as good color discrim- ination is concentrated in the central part of the visual field. Very rapid and precise movements of the eyes in mutual coordination are made possible by a set of three pairs of muscles attached to the outside of each eyeball. These muscles move the eye in all directions. Another set of muscles controls the lids, which in turn protect and lubricate the exposed surfaces of the eye. An added function of lid closure is as a part of the light adaptation process, i.e., the partial or Approved For Release 2 0 0 ' i'DJMXbP~ 8BO456OA007300010030-5 Approved For Release 2Oe . E c P78BO456OA00730001HBBO//-03/72 complete closure of the lids is one way to regulate the amount of light entering the eye, e.g., squinting in the presence of bright lights. The external shape of the eyeball is best described as that of two intersecting spheres - a small transparent one in the front and a large opaque one in the rear (see Figure 2.1). The front sphere, the CORNEA, holds a continually changing transparent fluid called the AQUEOUS HUMOR. The wall of the larger sphere has three specific layers: a tough light-proof outer protective coat, the SCLERA; a highly vascularized* nutrient layer, the CHOROID; and an innermost layer of complex and delicate nervous tissue called the RETINA. Within this sphere is a transparent gel, the VITREOUS HUMOR. Separating the two spheres is a curtain-like structure, the IRIS, with a slightly decentered** opening called the PUPIL. The size of this opening is continually changing due to the effects of illumination- level changes and other stimulus conditions. This control of the light level is also a part of the adaptation process of the eye. Directly in back of the iris and in the rear chamber is a small flexible transparent structure (the LENS). Its purpose is to vary the focal length of the eye to form a clear or focused image on the retina as the gaze is shifted to objects at different viewing distances. Muscular action by a structure surrounding the lens, the CILIARY BODY, exerts a force on the lens causing it to change shape and, hence, to alter its focal power. The alteration, known as the ACCOMODATION process (or focusing), is very important to the maintenance of clear imagery in the eye. Focusing ability declines with age--most people become far-sighted with age. These transparent struc- tures, the cornea, the aqueous humor, the lens, and the vitreous humor, comprise the focusing apparatus of the eye. Most of the focusing power of the eye is attributable to the cornea. The lens varies in shape and, hence, optical or focusing power, to permit the position of the image formed by the optics of the eye to fall on the retina. The innermost layer of the eye, the retina, is composed of nervous and supporting connective tissue, and it is structurally and functionally a forward extension of the brain. It spreads laterally over the entire inner surface at the back of the eyeball. However, its detail discrimination ability and color sensation are clustered in the MACULAR area, a depression in the retina, (see Figure 2.1) situated at'the extreme rear of the eye. Further, these abilities are strongest at the apex of the macula known as the FOVEA CENTRALIS. The optical quality of the image formed is also better at this point than anywhere else on the retinal * The choroid may also be thought of as an extremely dense mass of tiny blood vessels. ** This decentration is a partial basis for a little known illusion. (see Chromastereopsis, Section 2.4.3) that may have a bearing on the photointerpretation task. Approved For Release 20f9W)EMk*78BO456OA007300010030-5 Approved For Release 200tUopiDTA- 78BO456OA007300010 *R-03/72 Iris 1 \ \ Macular area Approved For Release 200f4DENM.78B04560A007300010030-5 Approved For Release 20 ffDfRe78BO456OA007300010a13 R-03/72 surface. The periphery of the retina does not have good discrimination for either detail or color. Its greater sensitivity to light, than that of the central retina, is achieved by adding weak signals from a wide area. Primarily, the peripheral retina is an aid in locating objects and in minimizing orientation, both of which must be understood as a part of the total visual process. The radial or in-depth arrangement of the retina with its 10 well-defined microscopic-layers, must be considered. The functional structure is "turned inside out", because the light must pass through the entire retinal structure before striking the light-sensitive or photo- receptive layer of the retina made up of the RODS and CONES (see Figure 2.2). In the rods and cones the light energy is absorbed and initiates chemical changes. The resulting "coded" electrical impulses flow along nerve fibers toward the center of the eye, traveling across a series of three types of nerve cells. The junctions between these nerve cells are known as SYNAPSES. At this stage, indeed even before it has left the eye, the signal is in the initial stages of interpretative processing by the brain. The prevailing opinion as to the character of the cones and their geometric arrangement is that there are three separate types of cones, each maximally sensitive to a wavelength in the red, green, or blue spectral region. The cones are situated side by side, possibly randomly and possibly clustered to a certain extent, but all at the same depth. The mechanism for the separation of the light energies into color is not well known, other than that it is a function of the cones. The function of the rods has nothing to do with discrimination of colors. The innermost layer of the retina, the GANGLION FIBER (see Figure 2.2) layer, converges from all lateral directions of the retina to a central exit point, the OPTIC DISC or BLIND SPOT. Here, all of the impulses leave the eye in a flexible cable-like structure, the OPTIC NERVE. The area of the optic nerve exit, since it has no rods or cones (the light receptors), is a true blind spot. The individual is unaware of this, because of two filling-in processes; one, a "mental" process and the other an overlapping by the corresponding image from the other eye. It is possible, however, that with prolonged staring with one eye (with the other eye covered or not being used for some reason) an object could be "lost" because its ocular image was focused on the blind spot. After leaving the eye, the optic nerves from the two eyes converge in an "X" shaped intersection, (see Figure 2.3) where half of the fibers within the nerve cross to the opposite side and half do not. The bundles of nerve fibers after this crossing are the OPTIC TRACTS. Thus, the fibers from the nasal side of the retina of each eye join with the temporal fibers from the retina of the other eye. All of the fibers from one side of the visual field go to one side of the brain. The individual Approved For Release 2006QNf4DbNTF L78BO456OA007300010030-5 Approved For Release 200tONFI:BT*1TtW8BO456OA007300010OW6/R-03/72 Direction of light Direction of impulses FIGURE 2.2 THE STRUCTURE OF THE HUMAN RETINA. 1, Pigment layer; 2, Rod and cone layer; 3, Synapses; 4, Bipolar cells; 5, Synapses; 6, Ganglion cells; 7, Optic nerve fibers. After Cady, F. E., and Dates, H. B., Illuminating Engineering, New York; John Wiley & Sons, Inc. 1928 (2nd Ed.), p. 233. Approved For Release 200 Rl gl6W8BO456OA007300010030-5 Approved For Release 20 O 1f)EWf J P78B04560A00730001*W#-03/72 Left Right ptic nerve Chiasma Geniculate body FIGURE 2.3 THE COURSE OF VISUAL STIMULI AND THE CORRESPONDING FIELDS OF VISION. Approved For Release 20 hW)WHP78B04560A007300010030-5 Approved For Release 200 N f I: IRpRf 8BO456OA00730001OQ 6/R-03/72 fibers contained in the optic tract at this point are the ganglion cell layer fibers from the eyeball. Some now branch off to the SUPERIOR COLLICULUS, but the majority go on to the region of the LATERAL GENICULATE BODY, a region of numerous synapses that constitute the first switching gap after the.eye. From the LATERAL GENICULATE, new nerve fibers, the OPTIC RADIATIONS, carry derivatives of the original signals from the retinal layer to the VISUAL CORTEX of the brain, at which point there is presumably some form of spatial analogue of the viewer's world. 2.2 THE COLOR SENSITIVITY OF THE VISUAL SYSTEM In a very strict physical sense, color does not exist. Color is an interpretation of the observer's visual impressions of an object or light source primarily related to the combinations of wavelengths of light energy being transmitted (as in a color image), reflected, or generated. As it happens, most people have similar interpretations, and, thus, color interpretations can be thought of as a common experience. People whose color discriminations and descriptions indicate the existence of very similar color vision are called COLOR NORMAL. The range of light intensities and colors that the color-normal eye is sensitive to are described below. 2.2.1 Luminous Range Although detection of the presence of light is possible at brightnesses as low as 10-6 cd/m2*, this is too dim for recognizing hue differences between (or within the area of) light sources. Such discriminations of hue difference do not begin until light intensities exceed 10-3 cd/m2. The first hue to be discriminated with an increase of intensity is red. (It also disappears last when luminance is decreased.) 'As luminance is increased, more colors gradually become distinguishable from each other, but not at the same time, i.e., light levels. Thus, there are two thresholds; the lower is the detection of the source as a light, and the higher is the identification of the source as a hue. The difference between the two thresholds is known as the PHOTOCHROMATIC INTERVAL. However, there is a separate value for each color. The graphical distribution of these values is shown in Figure 2.4. * Candelas per square meter. Approved For Release 2005QNf4 PE ATRDf478BO456OA007300010030-5 Approved For Release 20e f ib 11FIP78BO456OA007300011p W/1-03/72 _P4 luo 5Z0 ,:0 600 6i0 Wavelength, nanometers FIGURE 2.4 DISTRIBUTION OF THE FOVEAL PHOTOCHROMATIC INTERVAL. If the luminance continues to be raised, color eventually loses its purity or saturation, i.e., begins to wash out. Well below the level of this wash-out effect, a phenomenon known as the BEZOLD-BRUCKE EFFECT occurs. This is a perceived hue change (without an accompanying change in physical wavelength) with increasing luminance. The graph of this effect is shown in Figure 2.5. Note that the red-yellows and the green-yellows become yellower, and the red-blues and the green-blues become bluer. However, certain hues tend to remain constant or INVARIANT. In some instances, these are given as three distinct wavelengths and in others as four (Committee on Colorimetry, 1953). The former grouping is 478, 505, and 573 nanometers while the latter has been given as 474, 494 (complement), 506, and 571 nanometers. These are similar to but slightly different from the psychological primary hues (see below). They are also similar to and possibly related to the stable or INVARIABLE HUES, a phenomena related to the unchanging perception of a single hue as it falls on different parts of the retina (see 2.2.3 Geometric Extent of the Color Zones). Approved For Release 20C' IhIIDEK78BO456OA007300010030-5 Approved For Release 200tIMPIN 78BO456OA00730001O -6/R-03/72 r 75 650 625 800 57 5 550 525 SOO 475 4! Wavelength, nanometers FIGURE 2.5 BEZOLD-BRUCKE PHENOMENON. 2.2.2 Spectral Range The normal eye is sensitive to colors of wavelengths ranging from about 380 to 770 nanometers. However, at the extreme ends of this range the sensitivity to light energy is extremely low, i.e., the visual thresholds are very high. Consequently, the range is usually given as a working figure of 400-700 nanometers. This range of visual sensitivities makes up the VISUAL SPECTRUM, a very small part of the total electromagnetic spectrum (see 3.1 THE PHYSICAL ASPECTS OF COLOR). It is within this range that colors are seen and reliably, i.e., repeatedly with consistent results interpreted by humans with normal color vision. According to LeGrand (1968), 250 hues can be distinguished in side by side comparisons within the spectrum. Halsey and Chapanis (1951) find that this figure shrinks to about 11 if there is no reference or comparison chip immediately and simultaneously adjacent. Alternatively, if saturation and brightness are also allowed to vary, the number of distinguishable colors may reach into the millions. Table 2.1 is a breakdown of wavelengths by prevailing or popular names. These names can be further reduced to those of the psychological primaries, which Evans (1948) notes as blue, green, yellow, and red. These primaries have also been called the UNITARY (Judd, 1963) or UNIQUE HUES in that they seem to have no other contaminating colors. Combinations of these hues then can be used verbally to describe intermediate hues as in Table 2.1. Unfortunately, these names lack the precision (freedom from ambiguity) needed for other than verbal descriptions. Ingling (1971)*, for example, notes that the blue primary is between 470 and 480 nanometers; personal conversation Approved For Release 20031 AtPU&TR[9P78BO456OA007300010030-5 Approved For Release 2O E AtI P78BO456OA00730001H8l0